Open Collections

UBC Undergraduate Research

Effects of Ambient Noise on Affect and Perceived Napping Quality Han, Jiaojiao; Huang, Meihua; Niu, Tinyung; Ramsay, David; Quenneville, Vincent 2019-04-04

Your browser doesn't seem to have a PDF viewer, please download the PDF to view this item.

Notice for Google Chrome users:
If you are having trouble viewing or searching the PDF with Google Chrome, please download it here instead.

Item Metadata

Download

Media
18861-Han_J_et_al_PYSC_321_Ambient_noise_final_report.pdf [ 982.02kB ]
Metadata
JSON: 18861-1.0387181.json
JSON-LD: 18861-1.0387181-ld.json
RDF/XML (Pretty): 18861-1.0387181-rdf.xml
RDF/JSON: 18861-1.0387181-rdf.json
Turtle: 18861-1.0387181-turtle.txt
N-Triples: 18861-1.0387181-rdf-ntriples.txt
Original Record: 18861-1.0387181-source.json
Full Text
18861-1.0387181-fulltext.txt
Citation
18861-1.0387181.ris

Full Text

Running head: AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                                            1 UBC Social Ecological Economic Development Studies (SEEDS) Sustainability Program Student Research Report         Effects of Ambient Noise on Affect and Perceived Napping Quality Pentagon: Jiaojiao Han, Meihua Huang, Tinyung Niu, David Ramsay, Vincent Quenneville University of British Columbia PSYC 321 Themes: Buildings, Health, Wellbeing April 4, 2019         Disclaimer: “UBC SEEDS Sustainability Program provides students with the opportunity to share the findings of their studies, as well as their opinions, conclusions and recommendations with the UBC community. The reader should bear in mind that this is a student research project/report and is not an official document of UBC. Furthermore, readers should bear in mind that these reports may not reflect the current status of activities at UBC. We urge you to contact the research persons mentioned in a report or the SEEDS Sustainability Program representative about the current status of the subject matter of a project/report”. AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                2 Executive Summary  We examined what kind of sounds are relaxing to people, will help them sleep, and how they benefit their mood. Using a between-subjects design, participants listened to a short audio clip featuring either nature sounds, background noise from the UBC Student Union building, or sounds in the style of Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR) in order to compare their impact on mood, affect, and relaxation. We also measured the extent to which individuals enjoy and perceive these sounds as conducive to sleep. While we hypothesized that ASMR would be deemed most relaxing, calmness scores were highest amongst subjects in the nature sounds group, suggesting that sounds of this variety may be most likely to improve sleep quality. That said, there were no differences between groups in self-reported belief of the sounds’ sleep-conduciveness. Participants similarly liked ASMR and nature sounds, with background noises being the least favoured of the three. Our findings can help the design of UBC’s nap pods by confirming that even a one-minute intervention of nature sounds can serve as a possible sleep aid, and that the reduction of background noise would be beneficial in the nap pods to help improve sleep quality.  Keywords:  ambient noise, ASMR, nature sounds, background noise, perceived sleep quality, sleep, nap, sounds, relaxation AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                3 Introduction UBC is planning to install napping rooms or pods on campus to combat sleep deprivation, which is common among university student populations (Lund, Reider, Whiting, & Prichard, 2010; Oginska & Pokorski, 2006). The effects of sleep deprivation on college students include, among other things, reduced cognitive performance on cognitive tasks requiring critical thinking (Gaultney, 2010; Pilcher & Walters, 1997).  Following sleep deprivation, naps have been found to reduce fatigue and increase alertness (Brooks & Lack, 2006), improve mood, and have positive implications for cognition function and short-term memory (Bonnet, 1991; Lovato & Lack, 2010). Napping therefore not only presents a veritable solution to nocturnal sleep deprivation and the subsequent issues that come with it, but also a method by which individuals can more generally improve functioning in domains particularly relevant to university life. Numerous studies have shown that the majority of university students partake in napping (Evans, Cook, Cohen, Orne, & Orne, 1977; Lovato, Lack, & Wright, 2014; Pilcher, Michalowski, & Carrigan, 2001). The effects of background noise on nocturnal sleep quality have been studied extensively. These effects depend on various aspects relevant to the noise itself, to the individual, and to sleep architecture. Continuous traffic noises of 55 dBA and greater have been found to produce wakefulness and those of 45 dBA have been found to result in sleep disturbances (Eberhardt, Stråle, & Berlin, 1987). According to a recent meta-analysis, statistically significant increases in night time awakenings begin to occur at traffic noise volume levels as low as 33 dBA (Basner & McGuire, 2018). In one survey study of college students, “noise from others” was the most frequently endorsed reason for premature waking, and nearly half of respondents reported using sound as a sleep aid, the most popular being fans and music (Forquer, Camden, Gabriau, & Johnson, 2008). According to online survey data, 62% of respondents reported using music as a sleep aid, some citing a desire to tune out environmental noise (Trahan, Durrant, Müllensiefen, & Williamson, 2018). As is well-known, relaxation is key to sleep induction. It produces an atmosphere which activates the parasympathetic nervous system which in turn allows one to transition into a sleep state by releasing the neurotransmitter acetylcholine throughout the brain to induce physiological relaxation (Burgess, Trinder, Kim, & Luke, 1997). The current study explores which sounds will positively affect mood and perceived napping quality, thereby allowing students to relax and easily transition to sleep. We compare three sound conditions in this study: Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR), nature sounds, and background noise. Our first condition, Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR), is a phenomenon described as a “pleasurable and relaxing mental state triggered by various sounds in the environment” (Barratt & Davis, 2015). Of those who have experienced this phenomenon, some report a pleasurable “tingling” sensation which is said to further increase relaxation (Barratt & Davis, 2015). Research also claims that ASMR provides viewers with physiological and psychological benefits such as relief from chronic pain, muscle relaxation, reduced anxiety and reduced insomnia (Barratt & Davis, 2015). Supporting these claims, findings from a recent study suggest that for some individuals, exposure to ASMR videos results in increased positive affect and increased skin conductance levels, as well as simultaneous decreases in heart rate (Poerio, Blakey, Hostler, & Veltri, 2018). Importantly, many viewers of ASMR videos note that they often use the videos to fall asleep (Barratt & Davis, 2015). The deceptively unscientific term was coined as recently as 2010 (Cheadle, 2012; Richard, n.d.), and has gained considerable recognition by way of countless ASMR-inducing YouTube videos (Kovacevich & Huron, 2019).  AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                4 A video search on Google using the term “ASMR” returned more than 84 million results. Many of these involve actors (referred to as “ASMRtists”) who enlist a variety of tools to create various immersive soundscapes. These soundscapes include certain “trigger sounds” ranging from whispering, tapping on objects, typing, and hair-brushing, to performing various role-plays. Many viewers claim wearing headphones provides the optimal experience and full effects of the videos (Higham, 2014) since ASMRtists often use binaural recording technology to ensure an immersive experience (Tufnell, 2012). This, in turn, encourages viewers to pay close attention to the sounds. Our second condition, nature sounds, was chosen because many people use these sounds as sleep aids. This is because they activate the parasympathetic nervous system which leads to relaxation. In one study, participants with tinnitus-related sleep difficulties were given sound generators for nighttime use and asked to report on their sound preferences (Handscomb, 2006). The authors found that sounds of rain, a brook, the ocean, and birds were most preferred, and white noise and heartbeat sounds, least preferred. Saadatmand et al. (2013) found that when nature sounds were played in an intensive care unit (ICU) of a hospital, patients’ self-reported anxiety and agitation were reduced compared to a control group who received no intervention. Relatedly, ICU patients reported better overall sleep quality over two nights after listening to thirty minutes of nature sounds (Najafi Ghezeljeh & Nasari, 2018). Finally, we chose to use background noise at UBC nest as a control condition because students often nap on campus in open, public spaces. Barring intervention, designated napping areas on campus would likely be subject to similar noise.  In the present study, we used a between-subject design to examine what kind of sounds are relaxing to people, will help them sleep, and how sounds impact mood. Since little research has been done that compares ASMR and nature sounds, our goal is to test participants’ self-reported liking of the sound, perceived sleep quality after listening to the sound, and affective state after listening to ASMR, nature sounds or a control condition. We hypothesized that participants in the ASMR condition would report higher scores of liking, higher measures of perceived sleep quality, and a more positive affective state compared to those in the nature sounds and the control condition. The ultimate goal of this study is to determine what type of  sounds would be preferred in the napping rooms or pods.  Method Participants Participants were recruited through various social media pages frequented by UBC students (e.g. “UBC textbook exchange,” 2009) during 2019 March, where links to the study, hosted entirely on Qualtrics, were provided. One hundred twenty-two individuals completed the study, though only data from participants who reported being a UBC student were analysed. Participant characteristics are summarized in Table B1.  Conditions Our independent variable is the randomly assigned sound clip participants listened to, of which there were three levels: ASMR, nature sounds, and ambient noise at Nest (control condition). In the ASMR condition (n = 37), participants listened to the audio component of a YouTube video featuring slime being pulled over a microphone (Gentle Whispering ASMR, 2019). Participants in the nature sounds condition (n = 34) listened to the audio component of a YouTube video featuring a forest creek which included the sound of birds chirping AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                5 (johnnielawson, 2016), while those in the background noise condition (n = 46) listened to ambient noise recorded during lunch hour in the UBC student union building, the Nest. All sounds were set at an initial volume of 50 dBa.  Procedure  A between-subjects design was employed. Participants first finished a brief demographic questionnaire, and following this, were asked to wear either headphones or earbuds for the remainder of the study. Next, in an effort to maintain control and consistency over volume levels, participants heard the sound of a ringback tone1, were asked to adjust the volume on their computer or device until the sound was consistent with the volume at which they would normally hear it, and to keep the volume consistent for the remainder of the study. Then, participants were assigned to one of the three conditions using Qualtrics randomizer function. In each condition, participants were asked to listen to the sound for the full 59 s duration of the audio clip. Finally, participants were asked to respond to a number of questions measuring liking, perceived sleep quality, and affect/mood.   Measures Following the experimental session, participants responded to a variety of questions reflecting the dependent variables (see Appendix A). The extent to which participants liked the sound was measured by their rating on a five-point Likert scale answering “How much do you like the sound you just listened to?”. Expected effect of the sound on sleep quality was measured using five-point Likert scale ratings for two items: the first asked participants to indicate the extent to which they agreed with the statement, “I would sleep well with this noise in the background”, while the second asked participants to indicate the extent to which they agreed with the statement “I would not sleep well with this noise in the background”. The latter item was reverse-scored and summed with the former to provide an overall expected sleep quality score with higher values indicating greater expected sleep quality. Affect was measured with a modified version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), while mood and relaxation were assessed using a six-item mood scale adapted by Wilhelm and Schoebi (2007) from the Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire (Steyer, 2014). This latter instrument, as well as its adapted version used in the present study, measures mood along three dimensions: valence, energetic arousal, and calmness. For PANAS, we excluded questions not relevant to sleep to reflect our intention.  Results Differences between groups in terms of the seven dependent variables were assessed using one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) throughout. Significant effects were followed by Tukey’s honest significant difference (HSD) test. Results are summarized in Table B2.  Effect on Sleep and Liking The ANOVA on sound liking scores revealed a significant effect of condition, F(2,114) = 11.78, p < .001, η2 = 0.17. Post-hoc Tukey’s HSD showed that the mean liking scores in the background noise group (M = 2.24, SD = 1.23) were significantly lower than for the nature sounds condition (M = 3.46, SD = 1.05) and the ASMR condition (M = 3.00, SD = 1.08), the                                                  1 When using a landline, a ringback tone is the sound that indicates that the device or telephone associated with the dialed number is ringing (see Horak, 2009, p. 417). AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                6 latter two not being significantly different (see Figure B1). There were no significant differences between groups in terms of overall expected sleep quality, F(2,114) = 2.43, p = .092, η2 = .04—see Figure B2.  Affect and Mood As Figures B3 and B4 depict, there were no significant differences between groups in positive affect scores, F(2,114) = 0.82, p = 0.445, η2 = .01, or negative affect scores, F(2,114) = 0.10, p = .906, η2 = .00, respectively. In terms of the three mood dimensions, there were no significant differences between groups in valence (Figure B5), F(2,114) = 0.84, p = .436, η2 = .01, or energetic arousal (Figure B6), F(2,114) = 0.51, p = .604, η2 = .01. There was, however, a significant effect of group on calmness scores, F(2,114) = 6.96, p = .001, η2 = .11, with subjects in the nature sounds condition revealing higher scores (M = 9.98, SD = 2.28) than those in the background noise (M = 8.47, SD = 2.19) and ASMR (M = 8.16, SD = 2.68) groups, with the latter two differing by a non-significant amount (see Figure B7).  Discussion Our findings do not support our hypothesis that listening to ASMR sounds results in higher scores of liking, higher scores on measures of perceived sleep quality, and increased positive affect. Rather, our analysis lends support to the idea that nature sounds are more likely to induce relaxation, as reflected in group differences in calmness scores. Our results also suggest that individuals prefer ASMR and nature sounds to the ambient noise common in busy indoor environments. Although our initial assumption that ASMR would be most likely to benefit nap quality remains unfounded, we did find a relationship between people’s affective states and the sounds they are exposed to. In fact, the significant difference in liking and calmness were observed after participants listened to the sound for only one minute.  Ultimately, our results suggest that ASMR stimuli may not be as relaxing as claimed by Barratt & Davis (2015). In fact, participants in the ASMR condition reported similar levels of positive and negative affect relative to those in the other two conditions, and similar levels of calmness to those in the background noise condition. We suspect that this is at least partly because the sound of slime being pulled over a microphone was unfamiliar or strange to most participants, thereby requiring more cognitive resources to identify the sound as either threatening or relaxing. Another possible interpretation is that individuals vary in the extent to which they are susceptible to the effects of ASMR stimuli and that these individuals were not adequately represented in our sample. Nature sounds, on the other hand, had the unexpected effect of inducing the most relaxation. This could be due to the familiarity of the sounds. Alternatively, our results may support the biophilia hypothesis, which states that humans are biologically inclined to seek connections with the natural environment (Rogers, 2019). Finally, background noise was least liked and least likely to induce sleep among participants. This finding is in line with previous research showing that background noise interferes with sleep.      There are several limitations in our research given the scale and time we had to conduct the study. Firstly, our participants only listened to the sounds for one minute and did not nap during or following exposure to the stimuli. Secondly, we conducted surveys and used expected sleep quality and sound liking as indications of the effect of the sounds on actual sleep quality, which would be more precisely evaluated if individuals could report on actual sleep experiences with the sounds in the environment. It is also possible that extent to which participants liked the sounds, which was one of the significant differences between groups, do not mean people will AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                7 sleep better with the sound in the background as they nap. However, the higher levels of relaxation associated with nature sounds suggests that, compared with the other sounds tested in this study, nature sounds may contribute to higher-quality napping. Another challenge in this study is that despite ASMR having been found to be relaxing in previous research (Barratt & Davis, 2015), it may seem strange to individuals encountering it for the first time. As a result, we recommend that in future studies, researchers use more objective measures of sleep quality (e.g., physiological measures) after prolonged duration of listening to the sounds, as well as experiment with a wider variety of sounds. Additional research designs could include a longitudinal study following participants around for a few weeks after repeated sessions with nature sounds to see if its effects on liking and perceived sleep quality remain constant. Similarly, different groups could listen to nature sounds for varying durations to see if the effects discussed earlier are more pronounced with prolonged exposure to the sounds.  Recommendations for UBC client From our research, we have concluded that different sounds have differential effects on relaxation. Specifically, we found that students were more relaxed after listening to nature sounds. What’s more is that the effect was noticeable after listening to the sounds for only one minute. Considering these findings and that of participants’ relative dislike of noise from the busy indoor environment, we recommend implementing a gentle background nature sound in the napping environment. This is a low-cost, simple solution to benefit students’ napping quality. Also, given that we found different affective states reported by students after they listened to their respective sounds for one minute, an alternative option would be to have students listen to a relaxing sound on headphones inside a personal nap pod for a short duration to maximize relaxation and napping quality. This way, students may have the autonomy to choose to listen to a specific type of sound (or to opt out from listening altogether) and minimize noise in the napping area. In sum, we recommend using sound as a simple yet efficient tool to enhance students’ experience in the napping environment. In addition, our study confirms that the ambient noise in busy environments is unfavorable in promoting relaxation. We would recommend trying to reduce such noise in the napping area as much as possible. This may mean sound-proofing walls or nap pods, and possibly implementing a policy to remain silent upon entering the nap room.  Whether relaxing sounds (e.g., nature sounds) are played in the background or streamed individually, the present study lends evidence to the idea that certain sounds can positively impact sleep quality.  AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                8 References Barratt, E. L., & Davis, N. J. (2015). Autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR): A flow-like mental state. PeerJ, 3. doi:10.7717/peerj.851 Basner, M., & McGuire, S. (2018). WHO environmental noise guidelines for the european region: A systematic review on environmental noise and effects on sleep. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(3). doi:10.3390/ijerph15030519 Bonnet, M. H. (1991). The effect of varying prophylactic naps on performance, alertness and mood throughout a 52-hour continuous operation. Sleep, 14(4), 307–315. doi:10.1093/sleep/14.4.307 Brooks, A., & Lack, L. (2006). A brief afternoon nap following nocturnal sleep restriction: Which nap duration is most recuperative? Sleep, 29(6), 831–840. doi:10.1093/sleep/29.6.831 Burgess, H. J., Trinder, J., Kim, Y., & Luke, D. (1997). Sleep and circadian influences on cardiac autonomic nervous system activity. American Journal of Physiology-Heart and Circulatory Physiology, 273(4), H1761–H1768. doi:10.1152/ajpheart.1997.273.4.H1761 Cheadle, H. (2012, July 31). What is ASMR? That good tingly feeling no one can explain. VICE. Retrieved from http://www.vice.com Eberhardt, J. L., Stråle, L.-O., & Berlin, M. H. (1987). The influence of continuous and intermittent traffic noise on sleep. Journal of Sound and Vibration, 116(3), 445–464. doi:10.1016/S0022-460X(87)81376-7 Evans, F. J., Cook, M. R., Cohen, H. D., Orne, E. C., & Orne, M. T. (1977). Appetitive and replacement naps: EEG and behavior. Science, 197(4304), 687–689. doi:10.1126/science.17922 Forquer, L. M., Camden, A. E., Gabriau, K. M., & Johnson, M. C. (2008). Sleep patterns of college students at a public university. Journal of American College Health, 56(5), 563–565. doi:10.3200/JACH.56.5.563-565 Gaultney, J. F. (2010). The prevalence of sleep disorders in college students: Impact on academic performance. Journal of American College Health, 59(2), 91–7. doi:10.1080/07448481.2010.483708 Handscomb, L. (2006). Use of bedside sound generators by patients with tinnitus-related sleeping difficulty: Which sounds are preferred and why? Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 126(sup556), 59–63. doi:10.1080/03655230600895275 Higham, N. (2014, December 11). The videos which make their viewers “tingle.” BBC News. Retrieved from https://www.bbc.com/news/magazine-30412358 Horak, R. (2008). Ringback tone. Webster’s new world telecom dictionary (p. 417). Indianapolis, IN: Wiley. Retrieved from http://archive.org Kovacevich, A., & Huron, D. (2019). Two studies of autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR): The relationship between ASMR and music-induced frisson. Empirical Musicology Review, 13(1–2), 39–63. doi:10.18061/emr.v13i1-2.6012 Lovato, N., & Lack, L. (2010). The effects of napping on cognitive functioning. Progress in Brain Research, 185, 155–166. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00009-9 Lovato, N., Lack, L., & Wright, H. (2014). The napping behaviour of australian university students. PLoS One, 9(11). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0113666 Lund, H. G., Reider, B. D., Whiting, A. B., & Prichard, J. R. (2010). Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. Journal of AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                9 Adolescent Health, 46(2), 124–132. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.016 Najafi Ghezeljeh, T., & Nasari, M. (2018). Investigating the effect of listening to nature sounds on sleep quality in patients hospitalized in cardiac care units. Complementary Medicine Journal of Faculty of Nursing & Midwifery, 8(1), 2167–2180. Retrieved from http://cmja.arakmu.ac.ir/article-1-550-en.html Oginska, H., & Pokorski, J. (2006). Fatigue and mood correlates of sleep length in three age‐social groups: School children, students, and employees. Chronobiology International, 23(6), 1317–1328. doi:10.1080/07420520601089349 Pilcher, J. J., Michalowski, K. R., & Carrigan, R. D. (2001). The prevalence of daytime napping and its relationship to nighttime sleep. Behavioral Medicine, 27(2), 71–76. doi:10.1080/08964280109595773 Pilcher, J. J., & Walters, A. S. (1997). How sleep deprivation affects psychological variables related to college students’ cognitive performance. Journal of American College Health; Washington, 46(3), 121–126. doi:10.1080/07448489709595597 Poerio, G. L., Blakey, E., Hostler, T. J., & Veltri, T. (2018). More than a feeling: Autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR) is characterized by reliable changes in affect and physiology. PLOS ONE, 13(6), e0196645. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0196645 Richard, C. (n.d.). History of ASMR. Retrieved April 3, 2019, from https://asmruniversity.com/history-of-asmr/ Rogers, K. (2019). Biophilia hypothesis. Encyclopedia Britannica. Retrieved April 4, 2019, from https://www.britannica.com/science/biophilia-hypothesis Saadatmand, V., Rejeh, N., Heravi-Karimooi, M., Tadrisi, S. D., Zayeri, F., Vaismoradi, M., & Jasper, M. (2013). Effect of nature-based sounds’ intervention on agitation, anxiety, and stress in patients under mechanical ventilator support: A randomised controlled trial. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 50(7), 895–904. doi:10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2012.11.018 Trahan, T., Durrant, S. J., Müllensiefen, D., & Williamson, V. J. (2018). The music that helps people sleep and the reasons they believe it works: A mixed methods analysis of online survey reports. PLoS One, 13(11). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0206531 Tufnell, N. (2012, February 26). ASMR: Orgasms for your brain. HuffPost UK. Retrieved from http://www.huffingtonpost.co.uk UBC textbook exchange. (2009). [Closed group]. Retrieved from https://www.facebook.com/groups/ubc.textbook.4.sale/    AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                10 APPENDIX A Survey Questions Q1: How old are you? (e.g., 20)  Q2: What gender do you identify yourself as?  o Man  (1) o Woman  (2) o Non-binary  (3) o Prefer not to answer  (4)   Q3: What is your ethnicity/ethnic background? o Caucasian  (4) o Hispanic/Latino  (5) o African  (6) o East Asian (e.g., Chinese, Japanese, or Korean)  (7) o Southeast Asian (e.g., Vietnamese, Filipino)  (8) o South Asian (e.g., Indian)  (9) o Middle Eastern (e.g., Persian)  (10) o Aboriginal  (11) o Other (please specify):  (12) ________________________________________________   Q4: Which year are you in currently? o First year  (1) AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                11 o Second year  (2) o Third year  (3) o Fourth year  (4) o Fifth year or above  (5) o I am not a UBC student  (6)       Q5: In a typical week, how many naps do you take while on campus, excluding naps taken in residential facilities? o None  (1) o One  (2) o Two  (4) o Three  (5) o Four or more  (3)   Q6: Please listen to the following sound with earphones or headphones and adjust your volume through your electronic device to a level similar to how you would normally listen to the sound:  https://drive.google.com/open?id=1oaIaXj7Omvz4hH8YoS7sEdyXG2BNYtbT     Q7: What volume is your electronic device adjusted to according to the volume bar? o Lower than 20%  (4) o 20-49%  (5) o 50%  (6) AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                12 o 51-80%  (7) o Higher than 80%  (8)   Q8: What type of earphones or headphones did you use? o Earbuds  (4) o Over-ear headphones  (5) o Other (please specify)  (6) ________________________________________________   Q9A Condition 1: ASMR. Please listen to the following sound with earphones or headphones at the fixed volume until it stops.   https://youtu.be/NWKmQEnyt2s   Q9B Condition 2: Nature Sounds. Please listen to the following sound with earphones or headphones at the fixed volume until it stops.  https://youtu.be/ZmvRSAnkWAc   Q9C Condition 3: Background Noise. Please listen to the following sound with earphones or headphones at the fixed volume until it stops. https://youtu.be/zkwSMMX9i34   Q10: How much do you like the sound you just listened to? o Very Slightly or Not at All  (1) o A Little  (2) o Moderately  (3) o Quite a Bit  (4) o Extremely  (5)       Q11: Please answer the following questions according to the sound you just listened to for 1 minute. AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                13       Q12: How do you feel at this moment?   1 (1) 2 (2) 3 (3) 4 (4) 5 (5) 6 (6) 7 (7)   very tired o   o   o   o   o   o   o   very awake very content o   o   o   o   o   o   o   very discontent very agitated o   o   o   o   o   o   o   very calm not lonely at all o   o   o   o   o   o   o   very lonely full of energy o   o   o   o   o   o   o   without energy very unwell o   o   o   o   o   o   o   very well very relaxed o   o   o   o   o   o   o   very tense very socially disconnected o   o   o   o   o   o   o   very socially connected       AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                14   Q13: Please describe how much you agree with the following statements after listening to the sound:   Not at all (1) A little bit (2) Moderately (3) A lot (4) Very much so (5) I feel like I would sleep well with this noise in background (1) o   o   o   o   o   I feel like I would not sleep well with this noise in background (2) o   o   o   o   o      Q14: Please indicate to what extent you feel this way right now, that is, at the present moment.   Very Slightly or Not at All (1) A Little (2) Moderately (3) Quite a Bit (4) Extremely (5) Interested (1) o   o   o   o   o   Distressed (2) o   o   o   o   o   Excited (3) o   o   o   o   o   Upset (4) o   o   o   o   o   AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                15 Hostile (5) o   o   o   o   o   Enthusiastic (6) o   o   o   o   o   Proud (7) o   o   o   o   o   Irritable (8) o   o   o   o   o   Alert (9) o   o   o   o   o   Inspired (10) o   o   o   o   o   Nervous (11) o   o   o   o   o   Attentive (12) o   o   o   o   o   Jittery (13) o   o   o   o   o   Active (14) o   o   o   o   o   Afraid (15) o   o   o   o   o         AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                16 APPENDIX B Tables and Figures Table 1   Demographic Composition of Participants Demographics (N = 117) Age (SD) 20.84 ± 3.58 Gender   Female 78 (67) Male 38 (32) Non-binary 1 (1) Year   First 38 (32) Second 31 (26) Third 19 (16) Fourth 23 (20) Fifth, plus 6 (5) Campus Napsᵃ   Yes 56 (48) No 61 (52) Note. Values for categorical variables are presented as number (%). Values for the continuous variable (age) are presented as mean ± SD. atake naps on campus, not including those taken in residential facilities    AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                17  Table 2           Means, Standard Deviations, and One-Way Analyses of Variance (ANOVA) for Effects of Sound Condition on Seven Dependent Variables  ASMR Background Noise Nature Sounds ANOVA Post Hoc  (n = 37) (n = 34) (n = 46) F(2,114) p η²  Like 3.00 ± 1.08 2.24 ± 1.23 3.46 ± 1.05 11.78 < .001 .17 A, N > B SQ 6.05 ± 2.38 5.56 ± 2.43 6.67 ± 2.02 2.43 .092 .04 NS PANAS           PA 16.62 ± 5.55 18.24 ± 5.77 17.80 ± 5.46 0.82 .445 .01 NS NA 13.49 ± 6.09 13.18 ± 5.63 12.94 ± 5.24 0.10 .906 .00 NS Mood           V 8.35 ± 2.56 8.59 ± 1.79 9.02 ± 2.65 0.84 .436 .01 NS EA 6.78 ± 2.10 7.12 ± 2.36 6.63 ± 2.05 0.51 .604 .01 NS C 8.16 ± 2.68 8.47 ± 2.19 9.98 ± 2.28 6.96 .001 .11 N > A, B Note. Values in first three columns are presented as mean ± SD. Post hoc comparisons made using Tukey’s HSD, p < .05. SQ = sleep quality; PA = positive affect; NA = negative affect; V = valence; EA = energetic arousal; C = calmness; A = ASMR; B = background noise; N = nature sounds; NS = not significant; η² = effect size.               AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                18  Figure 1. Mean like scores (± SE) for each of the three groups. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.    AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                19  Figure 2. Mean sleep quality scores (± SE) for each of the three groups. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.      Figure 3. Mean positive affect scores (± SE) for each of the three groups. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                20  Figure 4. Mean negative affect scores (± SE) for each of the three groups. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.   AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                21  Figure 5. Mean valence scores (± SE) for each of the three groups. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.   AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                22  Figure 6. Mean energetic arousal scores (± SE) for each of the three groups. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.   AMBIENT NOISE AND NAPPING                23  Figure 7. Mean calmness scores (± SE) for each of the three groups. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.  

Cite

Citation Scheme:

        

Citations by CSL (citeproc-js)

Usage Statistics

Share

Embed

Customize your widget with the following options, then copy and paste the code below into the HTML of your page to embed this item in your website.
                        
                            <div id="ubcOpenCollectionsWidgetDisplay">
                            <script id="ubcOpenCollectionsWidget"
                            src="{[{embed.src}]}"
                            data-item="{[{embed.item}]}"
                            data-collection="{[{embed.collection}]}"
                            data-metadata="{[{embed.showMetadata}]}"
                            data-width="{[{embed.width}]}"
                            data-media="{[{embed.selectedMedia}]}"
                            async >
                            </script>
                            </div>
                        
                    
IIIF logo Our image viewer uses the IIIF 2.0 standard. To load this item in other compatible viewers, use this url:
https://iiif.library.ubc.ca/presentation/dsp.18861.1-0387181/manifest

Comment

Related Items